研究与报告
康养百科研究与报告

【学术研究】男性对嗅觉刺激的性反应

时间:2020-05-19 14:13:44     浏览量:2822     字号:

男性对嗅觉刺激的性反应

Alan R. Hirsch, M.D., F.A.C.P., and 

Jason J. Gruss

气味和味觉治疗研究基金会,芝加哥伊利诺伊州

摘要:

民间的智慧认为,各种香气都可以吸引人。但没有数据可证明特定气味对唤醒的实际影响。

本研究报告通过比较30种不同气味对31名男性志愿者的性唤起的影响,方法是通过戴有香味的口罩和戴无味的空白口罩对他们的阴茎血流量,肱动脉阴茎指数的影响。

本研究选择了在以前调查中发现的总体上令人满意的气味。每个都增加了一些阴茎血流量。薰衣草和南瓜派的混合气味产生了最大的增长(40%)。多种机制可以调节这些影响。在增味剂的潜在应用下,增加血管性阳痿的患者阴茎血流量值得研究。可能会减少阴茎血流量的气味尚未发现可用于治疗性变态者。


关键词:气味,性反应


介绍

历史上,某些气味被认为是壮阳药,这是许多民间传说和伪科学的主题。在庞贝的火山残骸中,香水罐保存在为性关系而设的室内。古埃及人沐浴时把精油作为指定用品;苏美尔人用香水吸引她们的女人。在中国传统仪式中,气味和性吸引之间的关系得到了强调,几乎所有文化都在结婚仪式中使用了香水。

在神话中,玫瑰花瓣象征着芬芳,作品“deflowering” 描述了性行为的最初表现。 在文艺复兴时期的意大利的流行即兴喜剧Commedia dell'Arte穿着长鼻子面具,象征着他们的阳具禀赋,对传统的冲击一直不能消散。

神分析已经在这些联系中取得了很多进展。佛里斯,在他的阴茎与鼻子的概念中,正式的描述了鼻子和阴茎之间的潜在联系。(1)荣格心理学也将气味和性链接起来。

在现代社会中,被用来商业推广和使用的香水、古龙水以及须后水都充满了浪漫诱惑的物品已经产生了数十亿美元的利益。(2)流行艺术也抓住了嗅觉和性之间的联系。电影《a Woman portrays》描绘了气味和性吸引在我们社会中的重要性,最近的小说《Perfumery》也如此。

在不同的历史时期和文化中,气味与性之间的特别联系暗示着进化重要性的高水平。弗洛依德(3)认为,气味是强烈的性情感诱因,以至于文明必须抑制气味。

解剖学揭示了气味和性别之间的联系:大脑中嗅觉的区域,嗅觉是边缘系统的一部分,即情感性大脑(4),是性思想和性爱产生的区域。(5)布里尔(6)谈到人们亲吻以使鼻子紧贴在一起,以便彼此嗅到(the Eskimo kiss)。或者他们可能会亲吻以使嘴巴合在一起,以便彼此品尝,因为我们所谓的味觉大部分取决于嗅觉。(7)

在讨论气味和性爱时,我们必须从鸟类和蜜蜂开始。传统上,蜜蜂,飞蛾和其他昆虫会释放出信息素,这些信息是吸引异性的雾化气味。(8)雌性飞蛾可以将信息素释放到空气中,从而吸引一公里内的雄性,从而增强了她的繁殖能力。同样,信息素在不仅在昆虫,乃至亚人类灵长类和灵长类属(9)中都存在,遍布了整个动物界,对物种进化具有重要意义。目前尚不清楚人类信息素是否存在,但理论基础支持它们的存在,因为整个动物界中存在的结构似乎也可能存在于人类中。在人脑内部,靠近鼻子顶部的是一个解剖学特征,这使我们有理由相信人类信息素存在:犁鼻器。(10)它的功能未知,但是在亚人类灵长类动物中,这是信息素起作用的区域以增加繁殖机会。这是人类犁骨蛋白结合的地方。(11)(12)

当我们运动时,我们会通过内分泌腺出汗。(13)但是当我们感到尴尬或性兴奋时,我们会通过顶泌腺出汗,这些顶腺会在腋下和生殖器周围释放高密度的类固醇(14);它们的角色是未知的。在亚人类的灵长类动物中,相同的顶泌腺释放信息素。(14)如果这些腺体在人类中的功能类似,这可能解释了为什么当女人高举手臂露出腋窝时,她的手势被认为具有性挑衅性;“这个迷人的洞穴充满了有趣的惊喜。” (13)

气味在性兴奋中的重要性在生理学证据有两个方面:第一,在性兴奋期间,鼻子充血会引起涡流的发展(像小龙卷风一样)。然后,由于较少的空气直接进入肺(15),所以更多的信息素或性引诱剂可以到达嗅觉上皮(16),并且气味更加尖锐。在性兴奋时从嘴里呼吸是鼻部充血的证据,并能最大限度地与兴奋剂和信息素接触。其次,女性的嗅觉能力(通常比男性(16-19岁)好)在排卵过程中达到顶峰,也许可以检测到存在的任何信息素。此时嗅觉能力增强可以解释为什么排卵期女性倾向于有更多的性经历。可能会增加嗅觉刺激,促使性活动增加。(20)

临床观察支持人类信息素的存在,这表现为大学室友效应(21-22)。同一宿舍的女生到了青春期以后开始有生理月经,经过一段时间以后,同一宿舍的女生的来月经的日期都会很相近。这表明一个女人释放的信息素可能会以主导的方式夹带其他女人。妇女在一起工作的小型办公室中也存在同样的现象。

作为信息素存在的进一步证据,要求男性大学生对戴着无味或假定的女性信息素(雄甾酮)的口罩时女性的照片进行评分。戴着假面罩的女性信息素的男人比起戴无味口罩的男人,她们看上去更友好,更漂亮(23)。

在英格兰学习期间,假定的男性信息素被放置在教室的某些桌子下面;然后连续拍摄照片以监控学生的坐姿。女学生倾向于坐在假定的雄性信息素所在的课桌旁(24)。当被问及为什么坐在那里时,女孩们说:“这似乎像是坐在正确的地方。”

信息素不仅可能是性吸引剂,而且可能是领土标记,例如,狗通过在院子里排尿来确立其统治地位(25)。在对男子大学宿舍的研究中,将假定的男性信息素放置在特定的厕所隔间下面,然后对其进行监控(24)。男人倾向于避开假定的雄性信息素所在的坑位,这似乎暗示着这种气味具有领土标记的作用。

这些实验当然不能证明存在人类信息素。然而,香水公司推销对信息素的解释,信息素通常含有麝香,即雄性麝香的信息素。玛丽莲·米格兰(Marilyn Miglan)将香水命名为“费洛蒙(Pheromone)”,但其香味是花香的混合物(26)。

各种文化偏爱各种气味。在美国,女性通常会剪下腋毛,因为她们认为这种体味被是不干净的。但是在东欧,这种气味被认为具有性刺激性,并且腋下毛囊是留给处女的。Alex Comfort称它为女人的花束(27)。

医学证据将气味和性反应联系起来。在一项研究中,超过17%的嗅觉缺陷患者出现了性功能障碍(28)。

嗅觉和性功能之间无疑存在关系。然而,其机制尚待发现。在本实验中,我们调查了环境嗅觉刺激对人类男性性反应的影响。


方法

参加者

通过经典摇滚电台广播征集招募了具有英语素养的受试者。31名年龄在18至64岁之间的男性自愿参加了这项活动。

措施

所有受试者进行嗅觉测试与宾夕法尼亚大学气味鉴别试验(UPSIT),共40项,被迫选择,划痕和嗅气味鉴别试验(29)和芝加哥嗅觉测试,三个气味检测和鉴定试验(30-32 )。他们被问及性偏爱,性行为和气味快感。

在实验过程中,根据制造商的规定(34),使用Floscope Ultra气胸镜检查仪测定肱的阴茎指数(33),以测定受试者的性唤起程度。使用该仪器,可以测量阴茎和肱动脉血压,并计算它们的比率,从而控制全身作用。这允许对阴茎血流进行特定的非侵入性评估。

步骤

选择了24种不同的气味,根据先前调查中总体上为享乐主意的男性进行此项研究。此外,从其中最受好评的2组中选择了6种组合。通过比较戴有气味的面罩的受试者的肱动脉阴茎指数与戴着无味空白面罩的受试者的平均指数来评估30种气味对阴茎血流的影响。针对每个对象针对每种气味进行此操作。

对受试者进行如下评估:将其阴茎在体积描记器上之后,让其适应三分钟,然后戴空白对照面罩一分钟,并记录肱动脉的阴茎指数。然后除去空白面罩,换上添加香气的面罩。因此,以双盲方式随机施用30个加香气的口罩,面罩之间间隔3分钟以防止对气味的习惯。每个面罩戴一分钟,记录肱动脉指数。最后,再加一个空白面膜一分钟,并再次记录肱动脉指数。

统计分析

统计显着性由ap值<= 0.05定义。数据分析包括以下非参数检验:符号秩检验,Wilcoxan秩和检验和Spearman秩相关系数(35-36)。


结果


结果

所有受试者都住在芝加哥或郊区。大多数(77%)是单身,平均年龄为30岁,中位年龄为29岁,范围为18至64岁。大多数(87%)是异性恋,有规律的性伴侣(74%),在实验前的30天内有四次性交,并认为他们的性生活还算令人满意(表1)。

为了评估其生理勃起功能,要求受试者以1(不存在)至5(每天早晨)的等级来评估早晨勃起的频率。他们的中位反应为3(表1)。大多数(84%)表示他们从未经历过气味诱发的勃起(表1)。

受试者的嗅觉特征示于表II。UPSIT分数是根据已发布的年龄和性别规范进行分级的。鉴于这些,52%的受试者得分是正常的,48%的得分是微观的,即低体(嗅觉敏感性不足)或厌氧(无嗅觉)。超过一半的受试者(55%)经历了气味诱发的回忆,这是一种香气触发记忆和相关感觉的现象(37)。超过一半(61%)是不吸烟者。大多数(71%)使用古龙水,在那些有定期性伴侣的人中,有83%的伴侣使用了香水。

气味对阴茎血流的影响

30种气味中的每一种都会增加阴茎血流量(表III)。薰衣草和南瓜饼的混合气味效果最大,使阴茎血流量中值增加了40%。其次是黑色甘草和甜甜圈的组合,使中位数阴茎血流量增加了31.5%。南瓜派和甜甜圈的总气味排在第三位,增加了20%。最少刺激的是蔓越莓,它使阴茎血流量增加了2%。没有气味减少阴茎血流量。

嗅觉低于正常的男性与嗅觉正常的男性无显着差异,吸烟者与不吸烟者也无显着差异。然而,在嗅觉能力正常的受试者中,有几个相关性是显着的:较高的肱动脉阴茎指数与年龄越大,对香草味的反应越大有关(p = 0.05);自我评估的性满意度水平与对草莓气味的更大反应相关(p = 0.05);性交的频率和频率与对薰衣草(p = 0.03),东方香料(p = 0.02)和可乐(p = 0.03)的气味有更大的反应相关。

讨论区

我们假设提升快感的性气味,因为它们具有其他行为影响(38-40),会增加阴茎的血流量。我们的数据支持这一假设。

存在多种可能发生这种情况的机制。这些气味可能引起巴甫洛夫式的条件反应,使受试者想起他们的性伴侣或他们最喜欢的食物(41)。在美国长大的人中,烘焙食品的气味最容易诱发一种被称为嗅觉诱发的回忆的状态(37)。在当前的研究中,气味可能引起怀旧的回忆,并伴有积极的情绪状态,从而影响了阴茎的血流(38-40)。或者气味可能只是放松。在另一些研究中,薰衣草向后增加阿尔法波,这种作用与放松状态有关(42-43)。在焦虑减轻的情况下,可以消除抑制作用,从而增加阴茎血流量。

已经显示出茉莉花的气味在正面增加了β波,这与机敏有关(42)。可能的气味可能会唤醒网状激活系统,使受试者对任何性暗示更加警觉,从而增加了阴茎的血流量。

气味的另一种可能是神经生理作用。MacLean(5)证明,刺激松鼠猴的中隔核可引起勃起。一条直接的途径将嗅球连接到中隔核(44),因此,从解剖学上看,气味可能会影响中隔核以诱导勃起并增加阴茎血流量,这在解剖学上是正确的。这似乎在我们的研究中很有可能,因为在整个实验过程中睡着的一位受试者对熏衣草和南瓜派的混合气味显示出最大的阴茎血流量增加。

我们怀疑是直接的生理机制,但是我们不能排除气味对睡眠中受试者梦境的可能影响,也许是梦境含量影响了阴茎血流。

通过隔核刺激,气味可能会增加攻击性。阴茎血流量的增加可能是诱发侵略的“邻里效应”的量度,而不是直接的性兴奋(45)。

我们也不能排除广义的副交感神经作用,它增加了阴茎血流量而不是特定的性兴奋(46)。为此,我们通过测量与阴茎血流同时发生的肱动脉血压来控制这一情况。

在我们的实验中,影响阴茎血液流动的特定气味主要是食物气味。更直接地,Rediwhip(c)已被用于女性生殖器,再次表明性别,食物和气味之间有很强的关系。这是否支持这一理论,即通往男人心脏(和性爱)的途径是通过他的胃?进化假设解释了为什么会这样。一次成功的狩猎之后,原始部落的人们聚集在食物周围(47)。在那里,也许他们拥有最多的机会。因此,响应食物气味而增加的阴茎血流量将是一个优势。关于黑猩猩的最新发现-当他们发现丰富的食物来源时,他们在进食前就停止了性行为,也许是为了减少对食物的争执,这为食物与性之间的关联提供了另一种解释(48)。

人类可以检测到大约10,000种气味(8)。研究表明,它们中的许多会影响行为,即某些花香可以增强学习(49)和购买行为(50)。青苹果的气味可能会减轻幽闭恐惧感(51),烧烤烟可能会引起飞行反应(51),吸入某些食物气味可能有助于减轻体重(52)。除本研究中检查的气味外,其他气味可能会对阴茎血流产生更大的影响。

嗅觉会影响性反射弧;如前所述,人类信息素是通过直接的嗅觉-边缘相互联系触发性反应的,是推测性的(53-55)。阴茎勃起是衡量男性性刺激的一种手段(56),是边缘系统中隔核和嗅觉纤维末端器官流出的一种表现(57)。根据自主神经系统的功能(58),阴茎充血由穿过阴部动脉和较小动脉至阴茎的动脉血流控制。性兴奋的第一个物理迹象是阴茎血流量的变化。进入阴茎的血液随着性兴奋而增加,而由于性抑制而减少(59)。

我们当然不能认为实验中的气味是人类信息素,因此我们认为它们是通过信息素以外的其他途径起作用的,而信息素被认为会对大脑产生内分泌作用。假定的信息素,雄烯醇(一种高密度的类固醇)对内分泌系统的作用非常缓慢(60)。与假定的信息素不同,影响阴茎血流的气味会立即作用于大脑或产生即时的心理影响。

这些初步数据表明,将气味作为治疗手段有潜在的用途。阳痿,有10%至15%的案例是器质性的,最常见的原因是血管性勃起功能障碍,通常是由糖尿病引起的(57-61)。当前的调查应确定无创气味治疗是否可以增强糖尿病患者的阴茎血流量。

尽管我们没有发现减少阴茎血液流动的气味,但我们推测可能会发现这种气味,可能是具有非常负面的抑制性气味的三叉神经兴奋剂。这种气味可以用来减少性恋者(如恋童癖者)的阴茎血流量,作为其适应或厌恶训练的一部分。

虽然我们仅研究男性受试者,但无疑会发现类似的气味会影响女性。正在进行阴道血流的平行研究。


● 资料来源


资料来源

1. Freud S.报告我在巴黎和柏林的学习情况(1886年)。弗莱斯论文的摘录。标准版,第一卷 1,伦敦,霍加斯,1892-99,173-280。2
. Dichter P.二手香水。药物与化妆品工业1995;156:72-75
3.弗洛伊德·S。《文明及其不满》(1930)。于:Strachey J,(编辑)。标准版,第一卷 21,伦敦,霍加斯,1961年,第100页
。4. MacLean PD。边缘系统及其海马结构:动物研究及其对人的可能应用。神经外科杂志(J.Neurosurg)1954;11:29-44
5. MacLean PD。脑部情绪的演变。在:刘易斯M,哈维兰JM(eds)。情感手册。纽约,吉尔福德,1993,66-67
6. Brill AA。神经症和精神病的嗅觉。Psychonanalyt Quart,1932年;1:7-42
7. Hirsch AR。闻起来和味道:烹饪专家与我们其他人相比。食品技术1990;44:96-102
8. Ackerman D.感官的自然历史。纽约,兰登书屋,1990年,5:
26-27。9. Durden-Smith J,deSimone D.性与大脑。纽约,阿伯大厦,1983年;216
10. Moran DT,Monti-Bloch L,Stensaas LJ,Berliner DL。人体犁鼻器器官的结构和功能。在:Doty RL(ed)。嗅觉和Gustation手册。纽约,Marcell Dekker,1995年,第794页。11
. Monti-Bloch L,Grosser BI。假定的信息素对人犁鼻器和嗅觉上皮的电活动的影响。类固醇生物化学杂志,1991年。39:573-582
12. Monti-Bloch L,Jennings-White C,Dolberg DS,Berliner DL。人类犁鼻系统。心理神经内分泌学1994; 19:673-686
13.高尔(Gower)DB,尼克松(Nixon)A,木槌AI。腋臭气味的重要性。于:Van Toller S,Dodd GH(eds)。香水:香水的心理学和生物学。纽约,查普曼和霍尔,1988,47,49
14. Russell MJ。人的嗅觉交流。自然1976; 260:520-522
15. Schneider RA,Wolf S.嗅觉活性对鼻膜功能的影响。J Appl Physiol 1960;15:914-920
16. Parlee MB。感觉过程中的月经:检查视力,嗅觉,听觉,味觉和触觉的波动。
17. Lanza DC,Clerico DM。人体鼻腔的解剖。于:Doty RL(ed)。嗅觉和Gustation手册。纽约,Marcel Dekker,1995年,第53页
。Hirsch AR。气味:嗅觉特征和异常。Internat J ARoma Ther 1992;4:16-17
19.认为DA,Doty RL。与年龄有关的苯乙醇气味检测阈值变化。Trans Penn Acad Ophthalmol耳鼻咽喉1987; 39:646-650
20. Adams DB,金牌AR,伯特。上升是女性在排卵时发起的性行为,并通过口服避孕药来抑制。N Engl J Med 1978; 299:1145-1150
21. McClintock M.月经同步与抑制。自然1979;229:244-245
22. Graham CA,McGrew WC。生活在男女同校校园中的女大学生的月经同步。心理神经内科,1980年;5:245-252
23. Kirk-Smith医学博士,展位DA,Carroll D,DaviesP。人类社会态度受雄甾烯醇的影响。Res Commun Psychol Psychiatr Behav 1978;3:379-384
24.古斯塔夫森(Gustavson)AR,道森(Dawson),博内特(DG)。推测的人类信息素雄甾烯醇会影响人类(智人)男性的选择性能。J Comp Psychol 1987;101:210-212
25. Ackerman D.感官的自然历史。纽约,兰登书屋,1990年,第26期。26
. Moran J.极佳的香水。加利福尼亚,新月大厦,1994,183
27.舒适度A.人类信息素的似然性。1971年《自然》;230:432-433
28. Hirsch AR,Trannel TJ。化学感觉功能障碍和精神病学诊断。1996年神经外科医学杂志(J Neurol Orthop Med Surg);17:25-30
29. Doty RL,Newhouse MG,Azzalina JD。宾夕法尼亚大学气味识别测试的内部一致性和短期重测信度。Chem Senses 1985;10:297-300
30. Hirsch AR,Cain DR。在正常人群中对芝加哥气味测试的评估[摘要]。Chem Senses 1992;17:
642-643 31. Hirsch AR,Gotway MB。主观常态神经系统疾病患者的芝加哥气味测试(CST)的验证[摘要] Chem Senses 1993; 18:570-571
32. Hirsch AR,Gotway MB。哈里斯AT。主观嗅觉丧失患者的芝加哥气味测试(CST)的有效性[Abstr。] Chem Senses 1993; 18:571
33. Laws DR,Pithers WD。阴茎体积描记器。在:Schwartz BK,Cellini HR(eds)。《治疗被关押的男性性罪犯的从业人员指南》。华盛顿特区,美国司法部,国家惩教所,1988年,第85-93页
。34. LifeSigns Corporation。PC兼容FLOSCOPE ULTRA。明尼苏达州明尼阿波利斯,血管实验室,1994年
35. Lehmann EL。非参数:基于等级的统计方法。纽约,Holden-Day,1975年
。36. Colton T.医学统计学。马萨诸塞州波士顿,小布朗,1974年
。37. Hirsch AR,怀旧:神经精神病学理解。消费者研究进展1992;19:390-395
38. Warm JS,Dember WN和Parasuraman R.视觉持续注意任务中嗅觉刺激对性能和压力的影响。J Soc Cosmetic Chemists 1991;42:119-210
39. Dunn C,Sleep J,Collett D.感觉有所改善:实验研究评估了重症监护室中芳香疗法,按摩和休息时间的使用。1995年高级护理杂志;21:34-40
40. RA男爵。环境诱导的积极影响:它对自我效能,任务绩效,谈判和冲突的影响。J Appl Soc Psychol 1990;20:368-384
41. RA男爵。心理学第三版。波士顿,马萨诸塞州,艾琳和培根,
1995,177 42. Sugano H.气味对心理功能的影响[摘要]。1988年 二十二:8
43.国王JR。使用香料减少焦虑。于:Van Toller S,Dodd GH(eds)。香水的心理学和生物学。伦敦,查普曼和霍尔,1988,157-16544。MacLean
PD,《大脑与行为的三位一体概念》。多伦多,多伦多大学,1973年,
1445。Donatucci CD,Lue TF。阴茎肿胀的精神病学。在:Hasmat AI,Das S(eds)中。阴茎 宾夕法尼亚州费城,Lea和Febiger,1993,1946。AdamsRD
,Victor M.《神经学原理》,第4版。纽约,McGraw-Hill,
1989,41846。AdamsRD,Victor M. Neurology,第四版。纽约,McGraw-Hill,
1989,41847。Diamond J.第三只黑猩猩:人类动物的进化与未来。纽约,哈珀·柯林斯(Harper Collins),1992,68
48. Blount BG。bo黑猩猩(pan paniscus)性行为问题。美国人类学家,1990年;92:702-71449。HirschAR
。约翰斯顿LH。气味和学习。1996年神经外科医学杂志(J Neurol Orthop Med Surg);17:119-126
50. Hirsch AR,盖伊。环境嗅觉刺激对普通消费品评估的影响。Chem Senses 1991;16:535
51. Hirsch AR,气味和对房间大小的感知[摘要]。第148届年会,精神病学协会,佛罗里达州,迈阿密,1995年
。52. Hirsch AR,Gomez R.通过吸入气味剂减轻体重。1995年神经外科医学杂志(J Neurol Orthop Med Surg 1995);16:28-31
53. Filsinger EE,法贝斯RA。气味交流,信息素和人类家庭。婚姻与家庭杂志1985;349年5月
54. Filsinger EE,Monte WC,Braun JJ,Linder DE。人(智人)对猪(sus scrofa)性信息素的反应5 alpha-androst-16-en-3-one,J Comp Psychol 1984; 98:219-222
55. Kirk-Smith M,Booth DA,Carroll D,DaviesP。受雄甾烯醇影响的人类社会态度。1978年心理学,精神病学和行为研究通讯;3:379-381
56. Hirsch MS,Melman A.阳of评估概述。在:Hashmat AI,Das S.(编辑)。阴茎 宾夕法尼亚州费城,Lea和Febiger,
1993,129-15357。KolodnyRC,Masters WH,Johnson VE。性医学教科书。马萨诸塞州波士顿,小布朗,1979,10-11,507-508
58。莱茵脑和边缘系统。讲义:库克郡医学院基础复习课程1981
59. Weiss医师,人类阴茎勃起的生理学。Ana Int Med 1972;76:793-79960。
古斯塔夫森(Gustavson)AR,道森(Dawson),博内特(DG)。推测的人类信息素雄甾烯醇会影响人类(智人)男性的选择性能。J Comp Psychol 1987;101:210-212
61. Pogach LM,Vaitukaitis JL。与勃起功能障碍有关的内分泌失调。在:克雷恩RJ,西罗基MVB,戈德斯坦一世(eds)。男性性功能障碍。马萨诸塞州波士顿,小布朗,1983,73





附表


附表1、31名男性受试者的性别特征

附表2、气味特征

附表3、各种气味对31位受试者产生的阴茎血流量增加







文章来源:美国神经外科和骨科医师学会

翻译:互联网


英文原稿

Smell and Taste Treatment Research Foundation, Chicago, Illinois

Abstract: Folk wisdom has it that various aromas are sexually enticing but no data exists demonstrating actual effects of specific odors on arousal. The
present study reports the effects of 30 different scents on sexual arousal of 31 male volunteers by comparing their penile blood flow, measured b brachial penile index, while wearing scented masks and while wearing nonodorized, blank masks. Odors found generally pleasant in previous surveys were selected for this study. Each produced some increase in penile blood flow; the combined odor of lavender and pumpkin pie produced the greatest increase (40%). A multitude of mechanisms may mediate these effects. A potential application of odorants to increase penile blood flow in patients with vasculogenic impotence deserves study. Odors that may decrease penile blood flow have yet to be found for possible use in treating sexual deviants.

Key Words: Odors, Sexual Response

Introduction

Historically, certain smells have been considered aphrodisiacs, a subject of much folklore and pseudoscience. In the volcanic remnants of Pompeii, perfume jars were preserved in the chambers designed for sexual relations. Ancient Egyptians bathed with essential oils in preparation for assignations; Sumarians seduced their women with perfumes. A relationship between smell and sexual attraction is emphasized in traditional Chinese rituals, and virtually all cultures have used perfume in their marriage rites. In mythology, rose petals symbolized scent, and the work “deflowering” describes the initial act of sex. Farcical stock characters in the popular Italian Commedia dell’Arte of the Renaissance wore long-nose masks to symbolize their phallic endowment, a tradition that lingers in the figure of Punch. Dramatic literature abounds with sly references to nasal size as symbolic of phallic size, as in the famous play Cyrano De Bergerac.

Psychoanalysis has made much of these associations. Fliess, in his concept of the phallic nose, formally described an underlying link between the nose and the phallus.(1) Jungian psychology also connects odors and sex.

In the modern world the pervasive promoting and use of perfumes, colognes and after-shaves as romantic enticements have produced a multibillion dollar business.(2) And the popular arts as well have seized on the theme linking olfaction and sex. The movie Scent of a Woman portrays the importance of smell and sexual attraction in our society, as does the recent novel Perfumery.

The prominent connection between odors and sex among diverse historical periods and cultures implies a high level of evolutionary importance. Freud (3) suggested that odors are such strong inducers of sexual feelings that repression of smell sensations is necessary to civilization.

Anatomy bears out the link between smells and sex: the area of the brain through which we experience smells, the olfactory lobe, is part of the limbic system, the emotional brain (4), the area through which sexual thoughts and desires are derived. (5) Brill (6) suggests that people kiss to get their noses close together, so that they can smell each other (the Eskimo kiss). Or possibly they kiss to get their mouths together so they can taste each other since most of what we call taste is dependent upon olfaction.(7)

In discussing odors and sex we must begin with the birds and the bees. Classically, bees, moths, and other insects are known to release pheromones, aerosolized odorants that attract the opposite sex.(8) A female moth can release a pheromone into the air that attracts a male s far as a mile away, enhancing her changes of procreation. Similarly, pheromones exist throughout the animal kingdom in insect, subhuman primate, and primate genera (9) to the evolutionary benefit of the species. Whether human pheromones exist is unclear, but theoretic grounds support their presence, since structures that exist throughout the animal kingdom seem likely to be present in humans as well. Inside the human brain, near the top of the nose is an anatomical feature that gives us reason to believe that human pheromones exist: the vomeronasal organ. (10) Its function is unknown, but in subhuman primates, this is the area where pheromones act to increase the chance of procreation. This is where human vomeropherins bind. (11) (12)

When we exercise, we sweat through endocrine glands. (13) But when we are embarrassed or sexually excited, we sweat through apocrine glands that release high-density steroids (14) under the arms and around the genitalia; their role is unknown. In subhuman primates, the same apocrine glands release pheromones. (14) If these glands function similarly in humans, this might explain why when a woman raises her arms to her head exposing her axillae, her gesture is considered sexually provocative; “this charming grotto is full on intriguing surprises.” (13)

Physiologic evidence of the importance of odors in sexual excitation is two-fold: First, during sexual excitation, engorgement of the nose induces development of eddy currents (like small tornadoes). Then, since less of the air goes directly to the lungs (15), more pheromones or sexual attractants can reach the olfactory epithelium (16) and smell is more acute. Breathing from the mouth during sexual excitation is evidence of nasal engorgement and maximizes contact with stimulants and pheromones. Second, olfactory ability in women, generally better than that of men (16-19) is at its peak during ovulation, perhaps to detect any pheromones present. Increased olfactory ability at this time may explain why periovulatory women tend to have more sexual experiences. Possibly increased olfactory stimulation prompts an increase in sexual activity. (20)

Clinical observation supports the existence of pheromones in humans, as manifested by college roommate effect (21-22). Women who move into all-women’s dormitory halls have, by mid-term, synchronized their ovulating cycles with the other women in the hall. This indicates that a pheromone released by one woman may entrain the others in a pattern of dominance. The same phenomenon exists in small offices where women work together.

As further evidence of pheromones’ existence, male college students were asked to rate pictures of women while wearing masks either with no odor or with a postulated female pheromone (androsterone). The men with postulated female pheromone in their masks described the women as appearing friendlier and prettier than did those wearing unodorized masks (23).

During a study in England, a possible male pheromone was placed beneath certain desks in a classroom; then pictures were taken continually to monitor where students sat. Female students tended to sit near the desks where the postulated male pheromone was placed (24). Asked why they sat there, the girls said “it just seemed like the right place to sit.”

Pheromones may be not only sexual attractants, but also territorial markers, e.g., a dog establishes dominance in his yard by urinating there (25). In a study of a men’s college dormitory room, a postulated male pheromone was placed beneath specific toilet stalls which were then monitored (24). Men tended to avoid the stalls where the postulated male pheromone was placed, which seems suggestive that the scent had the effect of a territorial marker.

These experiments, of course, do not prove human pheromones exist. Yet perfume companies market their interpretations of pheromones, often containing musk, a pheromone of the male musk deer. Marilyn Miglan named a perfume “Pheromone,” however its scent is a floral mixture (26).

Various cultures favor various odors. In the U.S., women cut their axillary hair because this bodily smell is considered unclean. But in Eastern Europe, the smell is considered sexually provocative, and the axillary follicles are left virginal. Alex Comfort calls it the woman’s bouquet (27).

Medical evidence links smell and sexual response. In one study, over 17 percent of patients with olfactory deficits had developed a sexual dysfunction (28).

A relationship undoubtedly exists between the olfactory and sexual functions; its mechanism, however, remains to be discovered. In the present experiment, we investigate the impact of ambient olfactory stimuli upon sexual response in the human male.

Method

Participants

Subjects literate in English were recruited through solicitation on classic rock radio broadcasts. Thirty-one males, aged 18 to 6 years volunteered.

Measures

All subjects underwent olfactory testing with the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test (UPSIT), a 40 item, forced choice, scratch and sniff odor identification test (29) and the Chicago Smell Test, a three odorant detection and identification test (30-32). They were queried as to sexual preference, sexual practices, and odor hedonics.

During the experiment, subjects’ sexual arousal was determined using the brachial penile index (33) with the Floscope Ultra Pneumoplethymosgraph following manufacturer’s protocol (34). With this instrument, both penile and brachial blood pressures were measured and their ratio calculated, hence controlling for systemic effects. This allowed specific noninvasive assessment of penile blood flow.

Procedure

Twenty-four different odorants were chosen for this study based on their generally positive hedonics in previous surveys. In addition, 6 combinations of 2 of the most well-liked of these were chosen. The effects of the 30 odors on penile blood flow were assessed by comparing a subject’s brachial penile index while wearing an odorized mask to his average index while wearing an unodorized blank mask. This was done for each subject for each odor.

Subjects underwent assessment as follows: after being attached to the plethysmograph, three minutes were allowed for acclimation, then a blank control mask was applied for one minute and brachial penile index recorded. The blank mask was then removed and an odorized mask applied. Thus 30 odorized masks were randomly applied in double-blind fashion, with a three minute hiatus between masks to prevent habituation to the odors. Each mask was worn for one minute and brachial penile index recorded. Finally, an additional blank mask was applied for one minute and brachial penile index again recorded.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical significance is defined by a p value <=0.05. Data analysis includes these nonparametric tests: Signed Rank test, Wilcoxan Rank Sum test, and Spearman's Rank correlation coefficient (35-36).

Results

Results

All subjects lived in Chicago or suburbs. Most (77%) were single and their mean age was 30 years, median 29 years with a range of 18 to 64 years. Most (87%) were heterosexual, had a regular sex partner (74%), had intercourse four times in the 30-day period just prior to the experiment and considered their sex lives fairly satisfactory (Table 1).

To assess their physiologic erectile function, subjects were asked to rate the frequency of their morning erections on a scale of 1 (absent) to 5 (every morning). Their median response was 3 (Table 1). Most (84%) stated that they had never experienced an odor-induced erection (Table 1).

Subjects’ olfactory characteristics are shown on Table II. UPSIT scores were graded based on published norms for age and sex. Given these, 52% of subjects scores were normal and 48% were microsmic, i.e., hyposmic (deficient in odor sensitivity) or anosmic (without a sense of smell). Over half the subjects (55%) had experienced odor-evoked recall, a phenomenon wherein an aroma triggers memories and associated feelings (37). More than half (61%) were nonsmokers. Most (71%) used cologne, and of those who had a regular sex partner, 83% of the partners used scent.

Effects of Odors on Penile Blood Flow

Each of the 30 odors produced an increase in penile blood flow (Table III). The combined odor of lavender and pumpkin pie had the greatest effect, increasing median penile-blood flow by 40%. Second in effectiveness was the combination of black licorice and doughnut, which increased the median penile-blood flow 31.5%. The combined odors of pumpkin pie and doughnut was third, with a 20% increase. Least stimulating was cranberry, which increased penile blood flow by 2%. None of the odors reduced penile-blood flow.

Men with below normal olfaction did not differ significantly from those with normal olfaction, nor did smokers differ significantly from nonsmokers. However, among subjects with normal olfactory ability, several correlations are significant: higher brachial penile indices correlate with greater age and with greater responses to the odor of vanilla (p=0.05); self-assessed level of sexual satisfaction correlates with greater responses to the odor of strawberry (p=0.05); and frequency of sexual intercourse correlates with greater responses to the odors of lavender (p=0.03), oriental spice (p=0.02) and cola (p=0.03).

Discussion

We hypothesized that hedonically positive odors, since they have other behavioral effects (38-40), would increase penile blood flow. Our data support this hypothesis.

A multitude of mechanisms exist by which this might occur. The odors could induce a Pavlovian conditioned response reminding subjects of their sexual partners or their favorite foods (41). Among persons raised in the United States, odors of baked goods are most apt to induce a state called olfactory-evoked recall (37). Possibly, odors in the current study evoked a nostalgic recall with an associated positive mood state that affected penile blood flow (38-40). Or the odors may simply be relaxing. In others studies, lavender, which increased alpha waves posteriorly, an effect associated with a relaxed state (42-43). In a condition of reduced anxiety, inhibitions may be removed and thus penile blood flow increased.

It has been shown that the odor of jasmine increases beta waves frontally, which is associated with alertness (42). Possibly odors may awaken the reticular activating system, making subjects more alert to any sexual cues, thus increasing penile blood flow.

Another possibility, odors may act neurophysiologically. MacLean (5) demonstrated that stimulation of the septal nucleus in the squirrel monkey induces erection. A direct pathway connects the olfactory bulb to the septal nucleus (44), hence, it seems anatomically correct that odor could impact upon the septal nucleus to induce erection with increased penile blood flow. This seems a strong possibility in our study, since the one subject who slept through the entire experiment showed the greatest increase in penile-blood flow in response to the combined odors of lavender and pumpkin pie.

We suspect a direct physiologic mechanism, yet we cannot rule out a possible impact of odors upon the dreams of the subject who slept through the experiment, perhaps with his dream content influencing penile blood flow.

Possibly odors can increase aggression, through septal nucleus stimulation. Increased penile-blood flow may be a measure of a “neighborhood effect” of induced aggression rather than direct sexual excitation (45).

Nor can we rule out a generalized parasympathetic effect, increasing penile blood flow rather than specific sexual excitation (46). As much as possible, we controlled for this by measuring brachial blood pressure coincident with penile blood flow.

The specific odors that affected penile blood flow in our experiment were primarily food odors. More directly, Rediwhip (c) has been used perigenitally, again indicating a strong relationship between sex, food and smell. Does this support the axiom that the way to a man’s heart (and sexual affection) is through his stomach? An evolutionary hypothesis explains why this may be so. After a successful hunt, humans in primitive tribes congregated around the food (47). There, perhaps they had most opportuni-ties to procreate. An increase in penile-blood flow in response to food odors, then would be an advantage. A recent finding about the Bonobos-that when they found a plentiful food source they stopped to have sex before they ate, perhaps to reduce quarreling over food-provides another explanation for the association between food and sex (48).

Humans can detect approximately 10,000 odors (8). Studies indicate that many of them affect behavior, i.e., certain floral smells can enhance learning (49) and buying behavior (50); green apple odor may ease claustrophobic feelings (51), barbecue smoke may induce a flight response (51) and inhaling certain food odors may help effect weight loss (52). Odors other than those examined in this study could possibly have a greater effect on penile-blood flow.

Olfactory sensation can influence the sexual reflex arc; as mentioned, human pheromones, which trigger sexual response through direct olfactory-limbic interconnections, are speculative (53-55). Penile erection, the measure of male sexual arousal (56) is a manifestation of outflow from the septal nuclei within the limbic system, and end organ for olfactory fibers (57). As a function of of the autonomic nervous system (58), penile engorgement is controlled by arterial flow through the pudendal artery and the smaller arteries to the penis. The first physical sign of sexual excitation is a change in penile-blood flow. Blood flow to the penis increases with sexual excitement and decreases with sexual inhibition (59).

We certainly cannot consider the odors in our experiment to be human pheromones, therefore we believe they acted through other pathways than do pheromones, which are thought to cause an endocrinologic effect upon the brain. A postulated pheromone, androstenol, a high-density steroid, is said to act very slowly on the endocrine system (60). Odors that affect penile-blood flow act immediately on the brain or have an immediate psychological effect, unlike the postulated pheromones.

These preliminary data suggest potential uses of odors as a treatment modality. Impotence, in 10-15% of cases, is organic, the most common cause being vaculogenic, usually due to diabetes (57-61). Current investigations should determine whether noninvasive treatment with odors can enhance penile blood flow in diabetes.

Although we found no odor to reduce penile blood flow, we hypothesized that such an odor might be found, possibly a trigeminal stimulant with a very negatively hedonic odor. Such an odor might be utilized to decrease penile blood flow in sex offenders, such as pedophiles, as part of their deconditioning or aversion training.

While we studied only male subjects, undoubtedly analogous odors might be found to affect women. Parallel studies of vaginal blood flow are being undertaken.

Refernces

1. Freud S. Report on my studies in Paris and Berlin (1886). Extracts from the Fleiss papers. Standard Edition, Vol. 1, London, Hogarth, 1892-99, 173-280
2. Dichter P. Second hand scents. Drug & Cosmetic Industry 1995; 156:72-75
3. Freud S. Civilization and Its Discontents (1930). In: Strachey J, (ed). Standard Edition, Vol. 21, London, Hogarth, 1961, 100
4. MacLean PD. Limbic system and its hippocampal formation: Studies in animals and their possible application to man. J. Neurosurg 1954; 11:29-44
5. MacLean PD. Cerebral evolution of emotion. In: Lewis M, Haviland JM (eds). Handbook of Emotions. New York, Guilford, 1993, 66-67
6. Brill AA. Sense of smell in the neuroses and psychoses. Psychonanalyt Quart 1932; 1:7-42
7. Hirsch AR. Smell and taste: how the culinary experts compare to the rest of us. Food Technology 1990; 44:96-102
8. Ackerman D. Natural history of the senses. New York, Random House, 1990, 5:26-27
9. Durden-Smith J, deSimone D. Sex and the brain. New York, Arbor House, 1983; 216
10. Moran DT, Monti-Bloch L, Stensaas LJ, Berliner DL. Structure and function of the human vomeronasal organ. In:Doty RL (ed). Handbook of Olfaction and Gustation. New York, Marcell Dekker, 1995, 794
11. Monti-Bloch L, Grosser BI. Effect of putative pheromones on the electrical activity of the human vomeronasal organ and olfactory epithelium. J Steroid Biochem Mol Biol 1991; 39:573-582
12. Monti-Bloch L, Jennings-White C, Dolberg DS, Berliner DL. Human vomeronasal system. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1994; 19:673-686
13. Gower DB, Nixon A, Mallet AI. Significance of odorous steriods in axillary odour. In: Van Toller S, Dodd GH (eds). Perfumery: Psychology and Biology of Fragrance. New York, Chapman and Hall, 1988, 47, 49
14. Russell MJ. Human olfactory communication. Nature 1976; 260:520-522
15. Schneider RA, Wolf S. Relatin of olfactory activity to nasal membrane function. J Appl Physiol 1960; 15:914-920
16. Parlee MB. Menstrual rhytms in sensory processes: Review of fluctuations in vision, olfaction, audition, taste, and touch.
17. Lanza DC, Clerico DM. Anatomy of the human nasal passages. In: Doty RL (ed). Handbook of Olfaction and Gustation. New York, Marcel Dekker, 1995, 53
18. Hirsch AR. Scentsation: Olfactory demographics and abnormalities. Internat J ARoma Ther 1992; 4:16-17
19. Deems DA, Doty RL. Age-related changes in the phenyl ethyl alcohol odor detection threshold. Trans Penn Acad Ophthalmol Otolaryngol 1987; 39:646-650
20. Adams DB, Gold AR, Burt AD. Rise is female-initiated sexual activity at ovulation and its suppression by oral contraceptives. N Engl J Med 1978;299:1145-1150
21. McClintock M. Menstrual synchrony and suppression. Nature 1979; 229:244-245
22. Graham CA, McGrew WC. Menstrual synchrony in female undergraduates living on a coeducational campus. Psychoneuroendocrino- logy 1980; 5:245-252
23. Kirk-Smith MD, Booth DA, Carroll D, Davies P. Human social attitudes affected by androstenol. Res Commun Psychol Psychiatr Behav 1978; 3:379-384
24. Gustavson AR, Dawson ME, Bonett DG. Androstenol, a putative human pheromone, affects human (Homo sapiens) male choice performance. J Comp Psychol 1987; 101:210-212
25. Ackerman D. Natural history of the senses. New York, Random House, 1990, 26
26. Moran J. Fabulous Fragrances. California, Crescent House, 1994, 183
27. Comfort A. Likelihood of human pheromones. Nature 1971; 230:432-433
28. Hirsch AR, Trannel TJ. Chemosensory dysfunction and psychiatric diagnoses. J Neurol Orthop Med Surg 1996; 17:25-30
29. Doty RL, Newhouse MG, Azzalina JD. Internal consistency and short-term test-retest reliability of the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test. Chem Senses 1985; 10:297-300
30. Hirsch AR, Cain DR. Evaluation of the Chicago Smell Test in a normal population [Abstr.]. Chem Senses 1992; 17:642-643
31. Hirsch AR, Gotway MB. Validation of the Chicago Smell Test (CST) in subjective normosmic neurologic patients [Abstr.] Chem Senses 1993; 18:570-571
32. Hirsch AR, Gotway MB. Harris AT. Validation of the Chicago Smell Test (CST) in patients with subjective olfactory loss [Abstr.] Chem Senses 1993; 18:571
33. Laws DR, Pithers WD. Penile plethysmograph. In: Schwartz BK, Cellini HR (eds). Practitioner’s Guide to Treating the Incarcerated Male Sex Offender. Washington, DC, U.S. Department of Justice, National Institute of Corrections, 1988, 85-93
34. LifeSigns Corporation. PC Compatible FLOSCOPE ULTRA. Minneapolis, MN, Vascular Lab, 1994
35. Lehmann EL. Nonparametrics: Statistical Methods Based on Ranks. New York, Holden-Day, 1975
36. Colton T. Statistics in Medicine. Boston, MA, Little Brown, 1974
37. Hirsch AR, Nostalgia: neuropsychiatric understanding. Advances in Consumer Research 1992; 19:390-395
38. Warm JS, Dember WN, Parasuraman R. Effects of olfactory stimulation on performance and stress in a visual sustained-attention task. J Soc Cosmetic Chemists 1991; 42:119-210
39. Dunn C, Sleep J, Collett D. Sensing an improvement: Experimental study to evaluate the use of aromatherapy, massage, and periods of rest in an intensive care unit. J Advanced Nursing 1995; 21:34-40
40. Baron RA. Environmentally induced positive effect: Its impact on self-efficacy, task performance, negotiation, and conflict. J Appl Soc Psychol 1990; 20:368-384
41. Baron RA. Psychology 3rd ed. Boston, MA, Allyn and Bacon, 1995, 177
42. Sugano H. Effects of odors on mental function [Abstr.]. JASTS 1988; XXII:8
43. King JR. Anxiety reduction using fragrances. In: Van Toller S, Dodd GH (eds). Psychology and Biology of Fragrance. London, Chapman and Hall, 1988, 157-165
44. MacLean PD, Triune Concept of the Brain and Behavior. Toronto, University of Toronto, 1973, 14
45. Donatucci CD, Lue TF. Psysiology of penile tumescence. In: Hasmat AI, Das S (eds). Penis. Philadelphia, PA, Lea and Febiger, 1993, 19
46. Adams RD, Victor M. Principles of Neurology, 4th ed. New York, McGraw-Hill, 1989, 418
46. Adams RD, Victor M. Principles of Neurology, 4th ed. New York, McGraw-Hill, 1989, 418
47. Diamond J. Third Chimpanzee: Evolution and Future of the Human Animal. New York, Harper Collins, 1992, 68
48. Blount BG. Issues in bonobo (pan paniscus) sexual behavior. American Anthropologist 1990; 92:702-714
49. Hirsch AR. Johnston LH. Odors and learning. J Neurol Orthop Med Surg 1996; 17:119-126
50. Hirsch AR, Gay . Effect of ambient olfactory stimuli on the evaluation of a common consumer product. Chem Senses 1991; 16:535
51. Hirsch AR, Odors and perception of room size [Abstr.]. 148th Annual Meeting Am Psychiatr Assoc, Miami, FL 1995
52. Hirsch AR, Gomez R. Weight reduction through inhalation of odorants. J Neurol Orthop Med Surg 1995; 16:28-31
53. Filsinger EE, Fabes RA. Odor communication, pheromones and human families. J Marriage and Family 1985; May, 349
54. Filsinger EE, Monte WC, Braun JJ, Linder DE. Human (homo sapiens) responses to the pig (sus scrofa) sex pheromone 5 alpha-androst-16-en-3-one, J Comp Psychol 1984; 98:219-222
55. Kirk-Smith M, Booth DA, Carroll D, Davies P. Human social attitudes affected by androstenol. Research Communications in Psychology, Psychiatry, and Behavior 1978; 3:379-381
56. Hirsch MS, Melman A. Overview of evaluation of impotence. In: Hashmat AI, Das S. (eds.). Penis. Philadelphia, PA, Lea and Febiger, 1993, 129-153
57. Kolodny RC, Masters WH, Johnson VE. Textbook of Sexual Medicine. Boston, MA, Little Brown, 1979, 10-11, 507-508
58. Cayaffa JJ. Rhinencephalon and the limbic system. Lecture notes: Cook County School of Medicine Basic Review Course 1981
59. Weiss MD, Physiology of human penile erection. Ana Int Med 1972; 76:793-799
60. Gustavson AR, Dawson ME, Bonett DG. Androstenol, a putative human pheromone, affects human (homo sapiens) male choice performance. J Comp Psychol 1987; 101:210-212
61. Pogach LM, Vaitukaitis JL. Endocrine disorders associated with erectile dysfunction. In: Krane RJ, Siroky MVB, Goldstein I (eds). Male Sexual Dysfunction. Boston, MA, Little Brown, 1983, 73



image.png



分享到:

首页 产品 购物车 我的